[go: nahoru, domu]

Gesture: Difference between revisions

Content deleted Content added
Added more information under typology, and created a new section named "Body Language Relating to Culture". Defined what manual gestures was and fixed some grammatical errors around the page.
Tags: nowiki added Visual edit
Line 31:
[[File:Unclesamwantyou.jpg|right|thumb|250px|[[Pointing]] at another person with an extended finger is considered rude in many cultures.]]
 
Humans have the ability to communicate through language, but they can also express through gestures. In particular, gestures can be transmitted through movements of body parts, face, and body expressions. <ref name=":12">{{Cite journal|last=Abner et. al.|first=Natasha|year=2015|title=Gesture for Linguists: A Handy Primer|url=https://goldin-meadow-lab.uchicago.edu/sites/goldin-meadow-lab.uchicago.edu/files/uploads/PDFs/2015_Abner,%20Cooperrider,%20Goldin-Meadow%20-%20Gesture%20for%20Linguists%20A%20Handy%20Primer.pdf|journal=Language and Linguistics Compass|volume=9|pages=437–449|doi=10.1111/lnc3.12168|via=Wiley & Sons}}</ref> Researchers Goldin Meadow and Brentari D. conducted a research in 2015 and conducted that communicating through sign language is no different from spoken language.<ref name=":5">''Gesture, sign and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies''. Available from: <nowiki>https://www.researchgate.net/publication/282760416_Gesture_sign_and_language_The_coming_of_age_of_sign_language_and_gesture_studies</nowiki> [accessed Nov 04 2018].</ref>
Although the scientific study of gesture is still in its infancy, some broad categories of gestures have been identified by researchers.
 
=== Communicative vs. informative ===
The first way to distinguish between categories of gesture is to differentiate between communicative gesture and informative gesture. While most gestures can be defined as possibly happening during the course of spoken utterances, the informative-communicative dichotomy focuses on intentionalityintentional of meaning and communication in co-speech gesture.<ref name=":1" />
 
==== Informative ====
Informative gestures are passive gestures that provide information about the speaker as a beingperson and not about what the speaker is trying to communicate.Some Examplesmovements ofare informativenot gesturespurely couldconsidered include such actions as scratching an itchgestures, adjustinghowever clothing,a orperson accessories,could orperform interactingthese withadapters objectin such asway takinglike ascratching, drinkadjusting orclothing, twirlingand atapping.'''<ref>Krauss, penR.M.; TheseChen, gesturesY. can& occur during speechChawla, butP. they(2000). may"Nonverbal alsobehavior occurand independently ofnonverbal communication,: asWhat theydo areconversational nothand agestures parttell of activeus?" communication(PDF). While''Advances informativein gesturesExperimental maySocial communicatePsychology''. information'''1''' about(2): the person speaking (e.g389–450. itchy,[[Digital uncomfortable,object etcidentifier|doi]]:10.1016/S0065-2601(08), this communication is not engaged with any language being produced by the person gesturing60241-5.</ref name=":1" />'''
 
These gestures can occur during speech, but they may also occur independently of communication, as they are not a part of active communication. While informative gestures may communicate information about the person speaking (e.g. itchy, uncomfortable, etc.), this communication is not engaged with any language being produced by the person gesturing.<ref name=":1" />
 
==== Communicative ====
Communicative gestures are gestures that are not informative. These are gestures that are produced intentionally and meaningfully by a person as a way of intensifying or modifying speech produced in the vocal tract (or with the hands in the case of sign languages), even though a speaker may not be actively aware that they are producing communicative gestures. The<ref previousname=":13">{{Cite examplesjournal|last=Abner et. al.|first=Natasha|year=2015|title=Gesture for Linguists: A Handy Primer|url=https://goldin-meadow-lab.uchicago.edu/sites/goldin-meadow-lab.uchicago.edu/files/uploads/PDFs/2015_Abner,%20Cooperrider,%20Goldin-Meadow%20-%20Gesture%20for%20Linguists%20A%20Handy%20Primer.pdf|journal=Language and Linguistics Compass|volume=9|pages=437–449|doi=10.1111/lnc3.12168|via=Wiley & Sons}}</ref> For instance, on the picture above of informativeUncle gesturesSam, canhe becomeis communicativepointing whenand sending a speakernon-verbal consciouslyform usesof themgesture by implying he wants you to communicatejoin somethingthe aboutU.S. themselvesArmy. orThis is a form of symbolic gesture, usually used in the absence of speech.  <ref name=":52">''Gesture, sign and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies''. Available from: <nowiki>https://www.researchgate.net/publication/282760416_Gesture_sign_and_language_The_coming_of_age_of_sign_language_and_gesture_studies</nowiki> [accessed Nov someone04 else2018].</ref>
 
<br />
 
== Body Language Relating to Gestures ==
Body language is a form of [[Nonverbal communication|non-verbal communication]] that allows visual cues that transmit messages without speaking. Gestures are movement that are made with the body: arms, hands, facial, etc. <ref name=":4">{{Cite journal|last=Tipper|first=Christine M.|last2=Signorini|first2=Giulia|last3=Grafton|first3=Scott T.|date=2015-08-21|title=Body language in the brain: constructing meaning from expressive movement|url=https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4543892/|journal=Frontiers in Human Neuroscience|volume=9|doi=10.3389/fnhum.2015.00450|issn=1662-5161|pmc=PMC4543892|pmid=26347635}}</ref> Authors Barbara Pease and Allan Pease, of the "[[The Definitive Book of Body Language]]" concluded that everyone does a shoulder shrug, a gesture signifying that the person is not comprehending what they are supposed to be understanding. Also, that showing the palms of both hands to show a person is not hiding anything, and raising the eyebrows to indicate a greeting.<ref>Pease, Allan, and Barbara Pease. ''The Definitive Book of Body Language''. , 2006. Print.</ref>
 
Finger gestures are commonly used in a variety of ways, from point at something to indicate that you want to show a person something to indicating a thumbs up to show everything is good. <ref>Black, Roxie M. (2011). "Cultural Considerations of Hand Use". ''Journal of Hand Therapy''. '''24''' (2): 104–111. [[Digital object identifier|doi]]:10.1016/j.jht.2010.09.067</ref>
 
Also, in most cultures nodding your head signifies "Yes", which the book "The Definitive Book of Body Language" describes as submissive gesture to representing the conversation is going the direction of the person speaking. Interesting, the book explains that people who are born deaf can show a form of submissive gesture to signify "Yes". <ref name=":4" />
 
== Manual vs. non-manual communicative gestures ==
Line 46 ⟶ 57:
 
=== Manual gestures ===
A gesture that is a form of communication in which bodily actions communicate particular messages. Manual gestures are most commonly broken down into four distinct categories: Symbolic (Emblematic), [[Deixis|Deictic]] (Indexical), Motor (Beat), and Lexical (Iconic)<ref name=":0" />It is important to note that manual gesture in the sense of communicative co-speech gesture does not include the gesture-signs of [[Sign language|Sign Language]]<nowiki/>s, even though sign language is communicative and primarily produced using the hands, because the gestures in [[Sign language|Sign Language]] are not used to intensify or modify the speech produced by the vocal tract, rather they communicate fully productive language through a method alternative to the [[vocal tract]].
 
It is important to note that manual gesture in the sense of communicative co-speech gesture does not include the gesture-signs of [[Sign language|Sign Language]]<nowiki/>s, even though sign language is communicative and primarily produced using the hands, because the gestures in [[Sign language|Sign Language]] are not used to intensify or modify the speech produced by the vocal tract, rather they communicate fully productive language through a method alternative to the [[vocal tract]].
 
==== Symbolic (emblematic) ====
Line 76 ⟶ 86:
Studies affirm a strong link between gesture typology and language development. Young children under the age of two seem to rely on pointing gestures to refer to objects that they do not know the names of. Once the words are learned, they eschewed those referential (pointing) gestures. One would think that the use of gesture would decrease as the child develops spoken language, but results reveal that gesture frequency increased as speaking frequency increased with age. There is, however, a change in gesture typology at different ages, suggesting a connection between gestures and language development. Children most often use pointing and adults rely more on iconic and beat gestures. As children begin producing sentence-like utterances, they also begin producing new kinds of gestures that adults use when speaking (iconics and beats). Evidence of this systematic organization of gesture is indicative of its association to language development.<ref name="mayberry"/>
 
Gestural languages such as [[American Sign Language]] and its regional siblings operate as complete natural languages that are gestural in modality. They should not be confused with [[finger spelling]], in which a set of emblematic gestures are used to represent a written alphabet. American sign language is different from gesturing in that concepts are modeled by certain hand motions or expressions and has a specific established structure while gesturing is more malleable and has no specific structure rather it supplements speech. We should note, that before an established sign language was created in Nicaragua after the 1970s, deaf communities would use "home signs" in order to communicate with each other. These home signs were not part of a unified language but were still used as familiar motions and expressions used within their family—still closely related to language rather than gestures with no specific structure.<ref>{{cite book|last=Fernandez|first=Eva M.|title=Fundamentals of Psycholinguistics|year=2011|publisher=Wiley-Blackwell|isbn=9781405191470|page=77|author2=Helen Smith Cairns}}</ref> This is similar to what has been observed in the gestural actions of chimpanzees. Gestures are used by these animals in place of verbal language, which is restricted in animals due to their lacking certain physiological and articulatoryarticulation abilities that humans have for speech. Corballis (2009) asserts that "our homininhominid ancestors were better pre-adapted to acquire language-like competence using manual gestures than using vocal sounds."<ref>Corballis, M. C. (2010), The gestural origins of language. WIREs Cogn Sci, 1: 2–7. {{DOI|10.1002/wcs.2}}</ref> This leads to a debate about whether humans, too, looked to gestures first as their modality of language in the early existence of the species. The function of gestures may have been a significant player in the evolution of language.
 
==Social significance==